Generic selectors
Exact matches only
Search in title
Search in content
Post Type Selectors
Search in posts
Search in pages
Filter by Categories
Author’ response
Author’s reply
Authors' response
Authors#x2019; response
Book Received
Book Review
Book Reviews
Centenary Review Article
Clinical Image
Clinical Images
Commentary
Communicable Diseases - Original Articles
Correspondence
Correspondence, Letter to Editor
Correspondences
Correspondences & Authors’ Responses
Corrigendum
Critique
Current Issue
Editorial
Errata
Erratum
Health Technology Innovation
IAA CONSENSUS DOCUMENT
Innovations
Letter to Editor
Malnutrition & Other Health Issues - Original Articles
Media & News
Notice of Retraction
Obituary
Original Article
Original Articles
Perspective
Perspectives
Policy
Policy Document
Policy Guidelines
Policy, Review Article
Policy: Correspondence
Policy: Editorial
Policy: Mapping Review
Policy: Original Article
Policy: Perspective
Policy: Process Paper
Policy: Scoping Review
Policy: Special Report
Policy: Systematic Review
Policy: Viewpoint
Practice
Practice: Authors’ response
Practice: Book Review
Practice: Clinical Image
Practice: Commentary
Practice: Correspondence
Practice: Letter to Editor
Practice: Obituary
Practice: Original Article
Practice: Pages From History of Medicine
Practice: Perspective
Practice: Review Article
Practice: Short Note
Practice: Short Paper
Practice: Special Report
Practice: Student IJMR
Practice: Systematic Review
Pratice, Original Article
Pratice, Review Article
Pratice, Short Paper
Programme
Programme, Correspondence, Letter to Editor
Programme: Commentary
Programme: Correspondence
Programme: Editorial
Programme: Original Article
Programme: Originial Article
Programme: Perspective
Programme: Rapid Review
Programme: Review Article
Programme: Short Paper
Programme: Special Report
Programme: Status Paper
Programme: Systematic Review
Programme: Viewpoint
Protocol
Research Correspondence
Retraction
Review Article
Short Paper
Special Opinion Paper
Special Report
Special Section Nutrition & Food Security
Status Paper
Status Report
Strategy
Student IJMR
Systematic Article
Systematic Review
Systematic Review & Meta-Analysis
Viewpoint
White Paper
Generic selectors
Exact matches only
Search in title
Search in content
Post Type Selectors
Search in posts
Search in pages
Filter by Categories
Author’ response
Author’s reply
Authors' response
Authors#x2019; response
Book Received
Book Review
Book Reviews
Centenary Review Article
Clinical Image
Clinical Images
Commentary
Communicable Diseases - Original Articles
Correspondence
Correspondence, Letter to Editor
Correspondences
Correspondences & Authors’ Responses
Corrigendum
Critique
Current Issue
Editorial
Errata
Erratum
Health Technology Innovation
IAA CONSENSUS DOCUMENT
Innovations
Letter to Editor
Malnutrition & Other Health Issues - Original Articles
Media & News
Notice of Retraction
Obituary
Original Article
Original Articles
Perspective
Perspectives
Policy
Policy Document
Policy Guidelines
Policy, Review Article
Policy: Correspondence
Policy: Editorial
Policy: Mapping Review
Policy: Original Article
Policy: Perspective
Policy: Process Paper
Policy: Scoping Review
Policy: Special Report
Policy: Systematic Review
Policy: Viewpoint
Practice
Practice: Authors’ response
Practice: Book Review
Practice: Clinical Image
Practice: Commentary
Practice: Correspondence
Practice: Letter to Editor
Practice: Obituary
Practice: Original Article
Practice: Pages From History of Medicine
Practice: Perspective
Practice: Review Article
Practice: Short Note
Practice: Short Paper
Practice: Special Report
Practice: Student IJMR
Practice: Systematic Review
Pratice, Original Article
Pratice, Review Article
Pratice, Short Paper
Programme
Programme, Correspondence, Letter to Editor
Programme: Commentary
Programme: Correspondence
Programme: Editorial
Programme: Original Article
Programme: Originial Article
Programme: Perspective
Programme: Rapid Review
Programme: Review Article
Programme: Short Paper
Programme: Special Report
Programme: Status Paper
Programme: Systematic Review
Programme: Viewpoint
Protocol
Research Correspondence
Retraction
Review Article
Short Paper
Special Opinion Paper
Special Report
Special Section Nutrition & Food Security
Status Paper
Status Report
Strategy
Student IJMR
Systematic Article
Systematic Review
Systematic Review & Meta-Analysis
Viewpoint
White Paper
View/Download PDF

Translate this page into:

Original Article
136 (
6
); 1025-1030

Inhibition of self-grooming induced by sleep restriction in dam rats

Department of Psychobiology-Universidede Federal São Paulo-São, Paulo, Brazil
Department of Physiological Sciences- Universidade Federal de Goiás - Goiânia, Brazil

Reprint requests: Dr Monica L. Andersen, Department of Psychobiology, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, Rua Napoleão de Barros, 925, Vila Clementino - SP 04021-002, São Paulo, Brazil e-mail: andersen.ml12@gmail.com

Licence

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-Share Alike 3.0 Unported, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Disclaimer:
This article was originally published by Medknow Publications & Media Pvt Ltd and was migrated to Scientific Scholar after the change of Publisher.

Abstract

Background & objectives:

Sleep restriction is a common feature of modern lifestyle and its effects can be extended to pregnancy. Several neurobehavioural consequences of sleep restriction during pregnancy have been reported, among which stand out perinatal depression and maternal fatigue, however, its effects over mother-infant relationship warrant further investigation. Thus, this study was aimed to evaluate the effects of sleep restriction during pregnancy over maternal behaviour and maternal aggression through animal models.

Methods:

Eighteen 90-day-old female Wistar rats were distributed in two groups: (i) Control - not submitted to any manipulation during pregnancy, and (ii) Sleep restriction - submitted to sleep restriction during the entire pregnancy (21 days) through the multiple platforms technique. In the postpartum day 5, resident-intruder paradigm and the latencies test were performed to assess both maternal behaviour and maternal aggression.

Results:

The sleep-restricted females displayed grooming in less frequency and duration, and with higher latency when compared to normal animals, while maternal aggression and maternal behaviour parameters remained equivalent between groups.

Interpretation & conclusions:

Considering the maintenance of maternal behavioural parameters, the inhibition of grooming seems to exert an adaptive mechanism, enabling sleep-restricted rats to display maternal behaviour properly.

Keywords

Grooming
maternal behavior
pregnancy
sleep restriction

Pregnancy is a physiological process and numerous physiological and anatomical changes take place in the body to sustain it. Even in normal conditions, this state predisposes women to lack of sleep, among other consequences13. During gestation, several sleep complaints and polysomnographic findings become common, ranging from hypersomnia and decreased slow-wave sleep (first trimester) to insomnia, non-restful sleep and diminished rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (third trimester)2. These complaints are mainly due to the marked anatomo-physiological changes inherent to this period in addition to alterations of hormone secretion, greater urinary flow and frequency, difficulty in assuming non habitual sleep positions, among other1.

The poor sleep quality related to pregnant women becomes more of a cause of concern when contextualized to modern living standards34. The lifestyle has changed affecting women more severely than man due to added social and professional demands5. This situation is aggravated during pregnancy, as pregnant women are subjected to insufficient sleep, promoted by intrinsic gestation circumstances and to impositions of modern lifestyle36.

Sleep curtailment in pregnant women has been associated with long-term impairment and negative outcomes after the parturition47. Sleep loss during pregnancy leads to neurobehavioural and psychiatric co-morbidities, including perinatal depression78 and maternal fatigue9. However, little has been documented about the postpartum consequences induced by sleep deprivation over the offspring as well as over parental behaviour. Recently, Pires et al4 have hypothesized that sleep impairment during pregnancy could lead to deficient mother-infant relationship. However, this concept warrants further investigation as it was theoretically discussed, but not experimentally assayed.

Animal models of maternal behaviour are useful experimental tools to understand the mechanisms underlying mother-infant relationship. However, the studies regarding the interface between maternal behaviour and sleep are incipient10. Thus, this study was aimed to examine the effects of sleep restriction during pregnancy over two important issues related to parental behaviour - maternal behaviour per se and maternal aggression in rats.

Material & Methods

This study was conducted in the Department of Psychobiology, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil. For this experiment 18, 90-day old female Wistar rats were obtained from the Center for Development of Experimental Models for Medicine and Biology (CEDEME), Sao Paulo, Brazil. These subjects were kept in monitored rooms with controlled temperature (22±1°C) and a 12 h light-dark cycle. The experimental protocol was approved by the institutional research ethics committee.

The female rats were mated, and pregnancy was confirmed by the presence of spermatozoa in the vaginal smear associated with compatible estrous cycle condition and, when possible, by the visualization of a vaginal plug. In the first day of pregnancy, the female rats were distributed in two groups: (i) Control - not submitted to any manipulation during pregnancy, and (ii) Sleep restriction – submitted to sleep restriction during the entire pregnancy (21 days). The control group for this experiment was in accordance to previous studies on maternal sleep deprivation through the platform method. Briefly, this protocol consisted of subjecting the animals to prolonged wakefulness for 18 h per day (from 1600 - 1000h) by the multiple platforms technique, for 21 days. During the remaining 6 h, the animals were returned to their home-cages and allowed to sleep. Sleep restriction was conducted in a tiled water tank (110×41×30cm), which contained 14 platforms, 6.5 cm in diameter and rising 1 cm above water surface11. The day of the partum was defined as postpartum day 0 (PPD0) and in the PPD1 the litters were standardized in 8 pups. The dams were subjected to behavioural tests to assess maternal behaviour and maternal aggressive behaviour at PPD3 and PPD5, which corresponds to the ascending phase of these behaviours1213. The protocol was adapted from earlier studies1415 and consisted of two different tests, as described below:

Resident-intruder paradigm: This test was performed to assess both aggressive maternal behaviour and maternal behaviour in the presence of an intruder rat. Thus, at the PPD3 and PPD5, a naive 60-day old male rat was introduced in the home-cage with the female rat and its respective litter. This test was able to quantify latency, frequency and duration of behavioral patterns related to maternal behaviour, maternal aggressive behaviour and non-maternal behaviours Table.

Table Results of comparison between control and sleep deprived females

Latencies test: This test was specific to maternal behaviour. Ten minutes after the end of the resident-intruder paradigm, the female rats were separated from their litter for 5 min. During this time, the litter was sent to the home-cage. When the female rat returned to the cage, the latency to retrieve the first and the last pup was measured. The time between the latencies was calculated.

Both behavioural tests had a cut-off of 10 min and were conducted in the first third of the dark phase. The tests performed at PPD3 were considered as habituation and were not quantified for analysis. The dams were weighed before the parturition and in the PPD5. The results of the tests at PPD5 as well as the dams’ weight were compared through the Mann-Whitney test.

Results

The most affected behaviour by sleep restriction during pregnancy was self-grooming, which was inhibited. Grooming was significantly reduced in the sleep restriction group regarding both frequency (4.7 ± 3.5 vs 1.6 ± 1.7; P=0.02) and duration (31.4 ± 24.7s vs 4.4 ± 5.3 sec; P<0.01) in relation to normal sleep animals. In addition, mean latency of self-grooming was significantly higher in sleep-restricted females (83.7 ± 66.4 sec vs 366.2 ± 225.9 sec; P<0.01), indicating that these animals delayed the drive to self-groom in the presence of a stressful stimulus (intruder male). Statistically significant results were also obtained in latency for lateral attack (168.7 ± 182.0 sec vs 71.33 ± 109.8 sec; P=0.04) and frequency for nest building (6.0 ± 6.3 vs 5.0 ± 3.7; P=0.03). There were no significant differences in dam's weight neither before the parturition (336 ± 21.2 vs 322.4±22.8; P=0.45) nor in the PPD5 (276.8 ± 28.1 vs 250.3 ± 15.4 g; P=0.07).

Discussion

The significant results in this study were found in the self-grooming. A decrease was found in lateral attack latency and in nest building frequency in sleep-restricted females, pointing to an increase in the defensive component of maternal aggression and decrease in a maternal care-related behaviour, respectively. The data regarding the lateral attack latency partially corroborated the results of Sandrin and Hoshino16, who have described an increase of defensive aggression in sleep-deprived rats. However, the findings related to nest building and lateral attack were isolated and not accompanied by complementary behavioural parameters (latency and duration), in contrast to the results for grooming. The hypothesis that the findings related to lateral attack and nest building were occasional and had no practical relevance can be raised, despite the statistical significance. Thus, both maternal aggression and maternal behaviour in sleep-restricted females were equivalent to the control group, as the majority of parameters related to these behaviours were not significantly different.

The sleep pattern in pregnant rats is similar to what is seen in women, showing dynamic oscillations in total sleep time and in other features17. Moreover, the sleep restriction methodology through the platform method is specific to REM sleep18, promoting sleep fragmentation rather than total sleep deprivation11. Accordingly, REM sleep deprivation provides a better framework to mimic disrupted sleep patterns during pregnancy than does total sleep deprivation because pregnant women show a marked REM sleep curtailment, mostly in the third trimester of gestation2. Hence, sleep restriction by the platform method in animals can be taken, with caution, as analogue to the sleep deficit scenario experienced by pregnant women. These results corroborate, in some degree, the hypothesis proposed by Pires et al4, since behavioral alterations related to mother-infant relationship were observed as consequence to sleep restriction. However, the behavioral alterations did not occur in maternal behavior per se, but in behavioral adaptive mechanisms, thus enabling effective maternal care.

The data were also analyzed in the perspective of the approach-avoidance model of maternal behavior1920. This model proposes that the maternal behaviour occurs when the tendency of approach and interaction with the litter is greater than the tendency to avoid pup stimuli. According to this model, maternal behaviour is a result of both maternal motivational and non-specific systems, the interaction between these systems, and the influence of external stimuli over them.

Self-grooming is a highly complex behaviour21 with a well known association with high stress situations22. In rodents, the grooming analysis is an indirect approach to infer about anxiety23. In general, the increase in self-grooming, mainly in frequency, composes the behavioural profile of rodents under an anxiogenic condition23. Considering the approach-avoidance model, anxiety-like behaviour and stressful conditions could be considere d as possible factors inducing the avoidance of the litter by the dam, or, more generally, anxiety and stress could compose the antagonistic system, avoiding the maternal behaviour as a whole. Since sleep deprivation and sleep restriction are closely related to stress2425, a clear deficit in maternal behaviour would be expected under these situations, however, it was not observed, suggesting an adaptive mechanism. The anxiolytic condition observed in the sleep restricted rats, evidenced by the increase in grooming latency and decrease in frequency and duration, could be responsible for allowing these females to display both maternal aggression and maternal behaviour properly.

The relationship between sleep and anxiety has been reported as a feature of adaptive behaviour mechanisms26. In the context of maternal coping, a marked stress hyporesponsiveness during lactation is described27 as well as a dramatic reduction in levels of anxiety-like behaviour28. These effects seem to be more prominent in the face of environmental challenges29. It might be possible that in an acute environmental challenge (the exposure to a male intruder in the resident-intruder paradigm), the anxiolytic conditions expected for lactating rats are even more prominent in those animals subjected to sleep restriction during pregnancy. Thus, the inhibition of self-grooming due to sleep restriction could be interpreted as one of the several adaptive mechanisms by which maternal care toward the litter is modulated in accordance with environmental demand30.

There are some methodological limitations in this study. First, one may inquire whether the results could be due to the stress promoted by sleep loss or to the sleep rebound that occurs after the sleep restriction protocol, rather than to sleep loss per se. However, it should be assumed that both sleep rebound and stress are conditionally associated with sleep deprivation. Even if one considers stress and sleep rebound as the causes of the described data, it is acceptable to state that these results are consequences of the context of sleep deprivation. Second, no sleep recording was performed in this study because the surgery and the recording equipment could represent additional sources of stress for the dams, leading to biased results. Also, the present results are strictly related to pregnancy and puerperium contexts and should not be extended to other conditions.

The present data should not be taken as conclusive findings, but rather as background to further investigation. Several aspects in this behaviour still warrant further studies, addressing in particular the extent to which grooming plays an adaptive role. Moreover, these studies could be even more useful if conducted with animal models that allow translational applicability. It must also be pointed out that the maternal bond and parental behaviour are far more complex in humans than in animals. Parental behaviour in humans is inserted into a social context that is not conceivable in animal experimentation. The adaptive responses observed in animals could give rise to neurobehavioural co-morbidities in humans, such as postpartum depression and maternal fatigue. Thus, the acquired data should be extrapolated to human behaviour with caution. Despite these considerations and of the preliminary nature of these findings, the results are of clinical importance regarding the relationship between disrupted sleep during pregnancy and mother-infant relationship.

References

  1. , , , , , , . Changes in sleep and sleep electroencephalogram during pregnancy. Sleep. 1994;17:576-82.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. , , , , , , . Sleep, rhythms and women's mood. Part I. Menstrual cycle, pregnancy and postpartum. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10:129-144.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. , , . Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3:519-28.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. , , , , . Sleep impairment during pregnancy: possible implications on mother-infant relationship. Med Hypotheses. 2010;75:578-82.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. , , . The social structure, stress, and women's health. Clin Obstet Gynecol. 2002;45:1099-118.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. , , . A longitudinal study of sleep stages in young women during pregnancy and postpartum. Sleep. 1992;15:449-53.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. , , , , . Sleep deprivation during pregnancy and maternal and fetal outcomes: is there a relationship? Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14:107-14.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. , , , . Fragmented maternal sleep is more strongly correlated with depressive symptoms than infant temperament at three months postpartum. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2009;12:229-37.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. , , . Relationship between child sleep disturbances and maternal sleep, mood, and parenting stress: a pilot study. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21:67-73.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. , , , . Sleep and behavioral research: overview on global trends and Brazilian relevance to this field. Sleep Sci (In Press)
    [Google Scholar]
  11. , , , . Comparison of the sleep pattern throughout a protocol of chronic sleep restriction induced by two methods of paradoxical sleep deprivation. Brain Res Bull. 2006;70:213-20.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. , , . Quantitative and comparative analyses of pro-aggressive actions of benzodiazepines in maternal aggression of rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1989;97:152-3.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. , , , , . Hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus, oxytocin, and maternal aggression in rats. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1997;807:606-9.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. , , , , . Dose-related effects of chronic gestational cocaine treatment on maternal aggression in rats on postpartum days 2, 3, and 5. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2001;25:1403-20.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. , , . On the dual nature of maternal aggression in rats. Aggres Behav. 1996;22:365-73.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. , , . Aggression in sleep deprived rats: ethological characterization of agnonistic confrontations as a defensive behavior. Rev Etologia. 1999;1:9-18.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. , , , . An animal model for pregnancy-associated sleep disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;52:209-11.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. , , . REM sleep loss increases brain excitability: role of noradrenaline and its mechanism of action. Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15:165-78.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. , . Motivational systems and the neural circuitry of maternal behavior in the rat. Dev Psychobiol. 2007;49:12-21.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. , , . An analysis of approach/withdrawal processes in the initiation of maternal behavior in the laboratory rat. In: , , , eds. Behavioral development. New York: Garland Press; . p. :177-230.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. , , . The grooming analysis algorithm discriminates between different levels of anxiety in rats: potential utility for neurobehavioural stress research. J Neurosci Methods. 2005;143:169-77.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. , , . Stress induced grooming in the rat - an endorphin mediated syndrome. Neurosci Lett. 1979;13:209-12.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. , , . Grooming analysis algorithm for neurobehavioural stress research. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc. 2004;13:151-8.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. , , , , . Different stress modalities result in distinct steroid hormone responses by male rats. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37:791-7.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. , , , , , . Paradoxical sleep deprivation activates hypothalamic nuclei that regulate food intake and stress response. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:1176-83.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. , , , . Stress-induced sleep rebound: adaptive behavior and mechanisms. Sleep Sci. 2009;2:151-60.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. , , , . Hypothalamic and limbic expression of CRF and vasopressin during lactation: implications for the control of ACTH secretion and stress hyporesponsiveness. Prog Brain Res. 2001;133:99-110.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. , . Alterations in behavioral and neuroendocrine stress coping strategies in pregnant, parturient and lactating rats. Prog Brain Res. 2001;133:143-52.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. , , , . Neuroendocrine and emotional changes in the post-partum period. Prog Brain Res. 2001;133:241-9.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. , , . Like mother, like daughter: evidence for non-genomic transmission of parental behavior and stress responsivity. Prog Brain Res. 2001;133:287-302.
    [Google Scholar]
Show Sections
Scroll to Top